Nevertheless, it is important to recognize that GRs play a
significant role in expressing meaningful distinctions, such as who is acting
upon whom, what is topical, and so on. A second important fact about GRs is
that they are essentially relational concepts. In other words, they don’t exist
unless there are two elements that are related. A nominal element by itself
does not “have” a grammatical relation. It is only when it occurs in a
structure with a verb that we can say that it is a “subject” or an “object,”
etc. In fact, it may be better to always say “subject of” or “object of” since
these terms make it clear that there must be another element in the
construction. The grammatical properties that identify GRs are determined by
syntactic constructions, and not simply by semantic properties of individual
nouns or verbs. Here is an analogy from real life.
A concept like “boy” is not
inherently relational, because it depends solely on the characteristics of the
individual. The concept of “brother,” on the other hand, is relational, because
someone can’t be simply a brother without reference to someone else. Getting
back to grammar, a category like plurality is non-relational, because it
usually depends on the semantic characteristics of the individual referent of a
noun. This semantic characteristic is reflected structurally in many languages
by some kind of “plural marking.” Subject, on the other hand, is a category
that depends on the structure of the whole clause.
A nominal element can only
be the “subject of” some other grammatical element. Sometimes the term argument
is used to refer to any nominal that has a grammatical relation to a verb, or
to another noun. This sense of the term “argument” is borrowed from mathematics
where an argument is an independent variable in a function; in other words, a
thing that has a property, or has a relation
to some other thing. A nominal that doesn’t have a
grammatical relation to some other word is called either a “non-argument,” or
an oblique. GRs can be reflected structurally by any number of features. The
three main structural features that often reflect grammatical relations in a
clause are the following: r Case marking on nouns r participant reference
marking on verbs (agreement, concord) r Constituent order In the following
pages, we will see examples of how different languages use these structural
features (and a few others) to organize systems of grammatical relations, and
will present some methods for analyzing them.
Grammatical relations within noun
phrases The simplest illustration of a grammatical relation is the genitive
relation that may hold between nouns in a noun phrase. In an English Determined
Noun Phrase (DP) like: (1) Caitlin’s quilt the word Caitlin’s refers to a
person the speaker is portraying as someone who, in some broad sense, is closely
associated with the quilt. Although we intuitively think of genitive arguments
(Caitlin in this example) as expressing “possession,” in fact the actual
relation between the message-world person referred to by the name Caitlin and
the message-world item referred to by the word quilt is in fact quite open
ended. The quilt may be the one that Caitlin made, e.g., in the context of a
contest in which homemade quilts are being judged, even though she already sold
it to someone else. Or it may be the quilt that Caitlin happens to be using
right now, though she is not its legal owner. It may be the quilt that Caitlin
just bought, or the one she likes best.
There are many examples of
genitive-plus-noun constructions in which the genitive noun cannot reasonably
be considered the “owner” of the other noun. For example: (2) a. Hiro’s
mathematics professor b. Milicent’s favorite political party c. the car’s color
d. Madaline’s home town e. the book’s main point Even though the semantic
relations between the genitive and the head noun are very different in all of
these examples, the morphosyntactic (grammatical) features that express the
relation in English are the same. Namely, the “possessor” comes before the head
noun and is marked by the suffix spelled ’s. These grammatical features
constitute evidence that the two nouns have a grammatical relation to one
another. This relation constrains, to a certain extent, the range of semantic
relations
0 comments:
Posting Komentar